You're walking down a busy city sidewalk, and you see someone in front of you fall down. What would you do? Now imagine that same situation, but you are the only other person on the sidewalk. What would you do then? According to social psychologists, you are more likely to help when there is no one else around. In contrast, if there are many witnesses, or bystanders, you might not offer help. It is even possible that no one would help the person at all. Psychologists believe this is a natural yet complex human reaction, which they call the bystander effect.
The bystander effect was first discovered in 1964 as a result of a very unfortunate event that happened outside Catherine Genovese's home in New York City. At three o'clock in the morning, someone attacked and murdered Genovese in front of her apartment building. The noise of the killing woke up 38 of Genovese's neighbors All of them looked out of their windows to see what was happening. However, not one of those 38 witnesses did anything to help. No one reported the murder to the police. The whole nation was shocked by the news the next day, and psychologists had no answers to explain why these people didn't help.
Newspapers called the 38 witness selfish and uncaring, but social psychologists John Darley and Bibb Latane had a different theory. They believed that a large number of witnesses actually decreased the chances tha any individual would help. If only one person witnesses a murder, he or she will feel fully responsible for calling the police. If there are two witnesses, each person might feel only half responsible. Now imagine there are many witnesses, as in the Genovese case. Darley and Latane pointed out that each person felt only a small amount of responsibility, so each did nothing. The reason they didn't help was not that they were uncaring or selfish people. There were just too many of them.
Darley and Latane knew they had to prove their theory scientifically, so they set up an experiment with college students to test it. They divided the students into three groups They took each student to a small building They put him or her in a room with a TV screen that showed another person in a different room in the building; then they left. Students in the first group thought that they were alone in the building. Students in the second group thought that there was one other person in the building Students in the third group thought that there were four other people in the building. As part of the experiment, the person on the TV screen pretended to become ill and called out for help. In the first group, where students believed they were the only people in the building, 85 percent went to get help for the person. In the second group, only 62 percent tried to help. In the third group, only 31 percent tried to help. The results supported Darley and Latane's theory. They figured out that having more witnesses did not mean that help was more likely. In fact, opposite was true.
You're walking down a busy city sidewalk, and you see someone in front of you fall down. What would you do? Now imagine that same situation, but you are the only other person on the sidewalk. What would you do then? According to social psychologists, you are more likely to help when there is no one else around. In contrast, if there are many witnesses, or bystanders, you might not offer help. It is even possible that no one would help the person at all. Psychologists believe this is a natural yet complex human reaction, which they call the bystander effect.The bystander effect was first discovered in 1964 as a result of a very unfortunate event that happened outside Catherine Genovese's home in New York City. At three o'clock in the morning, someone attacked and murdered Genovese in front of her apartment building. The noise of the killing woke up 38 of Genovese's neighbors All of them looked out of their windows to see what was happening. However, not one of those 38 witnesses did anything to help. No one reported the murder to the police. The whole nation was shocked by the news the next day, and psychologists had no answers to explain why these people didn't help.หนังสือพิมพ์เรียกว่าพยาน 38 เห็นแก่ตัว และ uncaring แต่นักจิตวิทยาสังคม Darley จอห์นและ Bibb Latane มีทฤษฎีอื่น พวกเขาเชื่อว่า พยานจำนวนมากท่าโอกาสที่บุคคลใด ๆ จะช่วยจริงลดลง ถ้ามีเพียงหนึ่งคนสืบพยานสังหาร เขาหรือเธอจะรู้สึกเต็มชอบเรียกตำรวจ ถ้ามีพยานสอง แต่ละคนอาจรู้สึกเพียงครึ่งหนึ่งที่รับผิดชอบ ตอนนี้สมมติมีพยานจำนวนมาก เช่นในกรณี Genovese Darley และ Latane ชี้ให้เห็นว่า แต่ละคนรู้สึกเพียงเล็กน้อยความรับผิดชอบ เพื่อละก็ เหตุผลที่พวกเขาไม่ได้ช่วยไม่ให้พวก uncaring หรือเห็นแก่ตัวคน มีเพียงจำนวนมากของพวกเขาDarley and Latane knew they had to prove their theory scientifically, so they set up an experiment with college students to test it. They divided the students into three groups They took each student to a small building They put him or her in a room with a TV screen that showed another person in a different room in the building; then they left. Students in the first group thought that they were alone in the building. Students in the second group thought that there was one other person in the building Students in the third group thought that there were four other people in the building. As part of the experiment, the person on the TV screen pretended to become ill and called out for help. In the first group, where students believed they were the only people in the building, 85 percent went to get help for the person. In the second group, only 62 percent tried to help. In the third group, only 31 percent tried to help. The results supported Darley and Latane's theory. They figured out that having more witnesses did not mean that help was more likely. In fact, opposite was true.
การแปล กรุณารอสักครู่..
